+ Site Statistics
+ Search Articles
+ PDF Full Text Service
How our service works
Request PDF Full Text
+ Follow Us
Follow on Facebook
Follow on Twitter
Follow on LinkedIn
+ Subscribe to Site Feeds
Most Shared
PDF Full Text
+ Translate
+ Recently Requested

Scaling relative incentive value: different adjustments to incentive downshift in pigeons and rats



Scaling relative incentive value: different adjustments to incentive downshift in pigeons and rats



Behavioural Processes 79(3): 182-188



Previous research suggests that pigeons and rats show differences in their behavioral adjustments in spaced-trial, incentive-downshift situations. Also, Papini and Pellegrini [Papini, M.R., Pellegrini, S., 2006. Scaling relative incentive value in consummatory behavior. Learn. Motiv. 37, 357-378] and Pellegrini and Papini [Pellegrini, S., Papini, M.R., 2007. Scaling relative incentive value in anticipatory behavior. Learn. Motiv. 38, 128-154] showed that changes in the rat's lever-pressing performance, runway running, and consumption of sucrose solutions after downshifts in incentive magnitude were a function of the ratio of postshift/preshift incentive magnitudes. Here, two experiments using a Pavlovian autoshaping procedure studied the adjustment of pigeons and rats to changes in incentive magnitude. In Experiment 1, pigeons received light-food pairings, whereas in Experiment 2, rats received lever-sucrose pairings. As a result, key-pecking and lever-pressing developed in each experiment, respectively. Preshift incentive magnitudes were downshifted so as to obtain postshift/preshift ratios of 0.125 and 0.25. Pigeons responded during the postshift phase according to the preshift incentive value and independently of the ratio value. However, rats showed ratio constancy, responding during the postshift in accordance with the postshift/preshift ratio, rather than with the absolute magnitudes of either the preshift or postshift incentives. These results support the comparative hypothesis that the mechanisms underlying ratio constancy during incentive downshifts are unique to mammals.

Please choose payment method:






(PDF emailed within 0-6 h: $19.90)

Accession: 023559546

Download citation: RISBibTeXText

PMID: 18755254

DOI: 10.1016/j.beproc.2008.07.008


Related references

Effects of testosterone administration and gonadectomy on incentive downshift and open field activity in rats. Physiology and Behavior 106(5): 657-663, 2012

Emotional Reactivity to Incentive Downshift in Adult Rats Exposed to Binge-Like Ethanol Exposure During Adolescence. Frontiers in Psychology 10: 315, 2019

Scaling relative incentive value in consummatory behavior. Learning and Motivation 37(4): 357-378, 2006

Scaling relative incentive value in anticipatory behavior. Learning and Motivation 38(2): 128-154, 2007

Role of the opioid system in incentive downshift situations. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 92(3): 439-450, 2009

Consummatory suppression due to incentive downshift is not a consequence of enhanced search behavior. Behavioural Processes 98: 69-71, 2014

Posttrial d-cycloserine enhances the emotional memory of an incentive downshift event. Behavioural Brain Research 223(2): 348-355, 2011

Long-lasting effects of perinatal asphyxia on exploration, memory and incentive downshift. International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience 29(6): 609-619, 2012

Posttrial corticosterone administration enhances the effects of incentive downshift: exploring the boundaries of this effect. Behavioral Neuroscience 123(1): 137-144, 2009

Impairment of recovery from incentive downshift after lesions of the anterior cingulate cortex: emotional or cognitive deficits?. Behavioral Neuroscience 125(6): 988-995, 2012

Emotional reactivity to incentive downshift as a correlated response to selection of high and low alcohol preferring mice and an influencing factor on ethanol intake. Alcohol 49(7): 657-664, 2016

Role of the ventrolateral orbital cortex and medial prefrontal cortex in incentive downshift situations. Behavioural Brain Research 244: 120-129, 2013

The Incentive Starting Line: Where to Start When Designing Your New Safety Incentive Program. Occupational Health and Safety 86(4): 47-49, 2018

Simple reaction time in children: Effects of incentive, incentive-shift and other training variables. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 13(3): 540-557, 1972

Differential dependence of Pavlovian incentive motivation and instrumental incentive learning processes on dopamine signaling. Learning and Memory 18(7): 475-483, 2011