+ Site Statistics
References:
54,258,434
Abstracts:
29,560,870
PMIDs:
28,072,757
+ Search Articles
+ Subscribe to Site Feeds
Most Shared
PDF Full Text
+ PDF Full Text
Request PDF Full Text
+ Follow Us
Follow on Facebook
Follow on Twitter
Follow on LinkedIn
+ Translate
+ Recently Requested

Knowledge inhibition and N400: a study with words that look like common words



Knowledge inhibition and N400: a study with words that look like common words



Brain and Language 62(2): 202-220



In addition to their own representations, low frequency words, such as BRIBE, can covertly activate the representations of higher frequency words they look like (e.g., BRIDE). Hence, look-alike words can activate knowledge that is incompatible with the knowledge corresponding to accurate representations. Comparatively, eccentric words, that is, low frequency words that do not look as much like higher frequency words, are less likely to activate incompatible knowledge. This study focuses on the hypothesis that the N400 component of the event-related potential reflects the inhibition of incompatible knowledge. This hypothesis predicts that look-alike words elicit N400s of greater amplitudes than eccentric words in conditions where incompatible knowledge is inhibited. Results from a single item lexical decision experiment are reported which support the inhibition hypothesis.

(PDF emailed within 0-6 h: $19.90)

Accession: 046508304

Download citation: RISBibTeXText

PMID: 9576822

DOI: 10.1006/brln.1997.1904


Related references

Knowledge inhibition and N400: a within- and a between-subjects study with distractor words. Brain Research 1187: 167-183, 2007

Knowledge of words, knowledge about words: Dimensions of vocabulary in first and second language learners in sixth grade. Reading and Writing 25(2): 347-373, 2012

The N400 is modulated by unconsciously perceived masked words: further evidence for an automatic spreading activation account of N400 priming effects. Brain Research. Cognitive Brain Research 13(1): 27-39, 2002

Common Words are Longer in Apparent Duration than are Rare Words. Journal of General Psychology 118(1): 83-95, 1991

Are the spoken durations of rare words longer than those of common words?. Memory & Cognition 11(1): 13-15, 1983

Equivalent inter- and intramodality long-term priming: evidence for a common lexicon for words seen and words heard. Memory and Cognition 35(4): 781-800, 2007

Phonotactic knowledge and lexical-semantic processing in one-year-olds: brain responses to words and nonsense words in picture contexts. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 17(11): 1785-1802, 2005

Lexical decision after left, right and bilateral presentation of function words, content words and non-words: Evidence for interhemispheric interaction. Neuropsychologia 32(1): 105-124, 1994

There is nothing rotten (with elderly STEMI care) in the (Western) State of Denmark: Polonius: what do you read, my lord? Hamlet: words, words, words. Catheterization and Cardiovascular Interventions 81(6): 920-921, 2013

Frequent words do not break continuous flash suppression differently from infrequent or nonexistent words: implications for semantic processing of words in the absence of awareness. Plos One 9(8): E104719, 2016

(Non)words, (non)words, (non)words: evidence for a protolexicon during the first year of life. Developmental Science 16(1): 24-34, 2013

Words, words, words: Even the parliamentarians are the very models of post-modernists. Children Australia 20(04): 38-39, 1995

"Words have no words for words that are not true": a rejoinder to Serge Tcherkezoff. Journal of the Polynesian Society. Polynesian Society 110(3): 301-311, 2008

N400 to semantically anomalous pictures and words. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 4(1): 15-22, 1992

Electrodermal responses to words in chronic low back pain patients: a comparison between pain descriptors, other emotional words, and neutral words. Clinical Journal of Pain 22(8): 686-691, 2006