+ Site Statistics
+ Search Articles
+ PDF Full Text Service
How our service works
Request PDF Full Text
+ Follow Us
Follow on Facebook
Follow on Twitter
Follow on LinkedIn
+ Subscribe to Site Feeds
Most Shared
PDF Full Text
+ Translate
+ Recently Requested

The contributions of static visual cues, nonvisual cues, and optic flow in distance estimation



The contributions of static visual cues, nonvisual cues, and optic flow in distance estimation



Perception 33(1): 49-65



By systematically varying cue availability in the stimulus and response phases of a series of same-modality and cross-modality distance matching tasks, we examined the contributions of static visual information, idiothetic information, and optic flow information. The experiment was conducted in a large-scale, open, outdoor environment. Subjects were presented with information about a distance and were then required to turn 180 before producing a distance estimate. Distance encoding and responding occurred via: (i) visually perceived target distance, or (ii) traversed distance through either blindfolded locomotion or during sighted locomotion. The results demonstrated that subjects performed with similar accuracy across all conditions. In conditions in which the stimulus and the response were delivered in the same mode, when visual information was absent, constant error was minimal; whereas, when visual information was present, overestimation was observed. In conditions in which the stimulus and response modes differed, a consistent error pattern was observed. By systematically comparing complementary conditions, we found that the availability of visual information during locomotion (particularly optic flow) led to an 'under-perception' of movement relative to conditions in which visual information was absent during locomotion.

Please choose payment method:






(PDF emailed within 0-6 h: $19.90)

Accession: 050592896

Download citation: RISBibTeXText

PMID: 15035328

DOI: 10.1068/p5145


Related references

Tactical decisions for changeable cuttlefish camouflage: visual cues for choosing masquerade are relevant from a greater distance than visual cues used for background matching. Biological Bulletin 229(2): 160-166, 2016

Visual perception of planar orientation: Dominance of static depth cues over motion cues. Vision Research 42(11): 1403-1412, 2002

Humans combine the optic flow with static depth cues for robust perception of heading. Vision Research. 34(16): 2153-2167, 1994

Cross-modal transfer in visual and nonvisual cues in bumblebees. Journal of Comparative Physiology. A, Neuroethology, Sensory, Neural, and Behavioral Physiology 2019, 2019

Accurate visual heading estimation at high rotation rate without oculomotor or static-depth cues. Society for Neuroscience Abstracts 21(1-3): 125, 1995

Are optic flow cues for neurons in the cat PMLS cortex similar across the visual field?. Society for Neuroscience Abstracts 25(1-2): 671, 1999

A model of the combination of optic flow and extraretinal eye movement signals in primate extrastriate visual cortex. Neural model of self-motion from optic flow and extraretinal cues. Neural Networks 11(3): 397-414, 2003

Olfactory and/or visual cues for spatial navigation through ontogeny: olfactory cues enable the use of visual cues. Behavioral Neuroscience 117(3): 412-425, 2003

Vestibular dominance in estimating the distance of self motion Misaligned optic flow and vestibular cues. IOVS 39(4): S1093, March 15, 1998

Vowel identification by cochlear implant users: Contributions of duration cues and dynamic spectral cues. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 138(1): 65-73, 2016

Olfactory cues are more effective than visual cues in experimentally triggering autobiographical memories. Memory 26(4): 547-558, 2017

Gaze in Visual Search Is Guided More Efficiently by Positive Cues than by Negative Cues. Plos One 10(12): E0145910, 2016

The relationship between central cues and peripheral cues in covert visual orientation. Perception & Psychophysics 59(6): 885-899, 1997

Audio cues enhance mirroring of arm motion when visual cues are scarce. Journal of the Royal Society, Interface 16(154): 20180903, 2019

Olfactory cues enable rats to use visual cues for place navigation in total darkness. European Journal of Neuroscience 12(Suppl. 11): 86, 2000